At First Glance…
We know that teaching whole-group style usually results
in “teaching to the middle”, leaving the more advanced or faster learners and
less advanced or slower learners behind.
We also know that children cannot be expected to have the same learning
styles, interests, motives, or background information and skills. At first glance, ability-based grouping seems
to be an effective way to meet all students’ unique needs, to teach students
social-emotional skills (self-concept, empathy, working cooperatively with
diverse others, etc.), and to close the achievement gap. If ability grouping works as it is intended
to, then all students should benefit
from it, right? After reviewing research
studies and opponents’ views on the effects of within-class ability grouping however,
its equitability must be questioned…
However…
Several
studies have found that within-class ability grouping actually creates increased social and educational
stratification and inequality, that it is “…elitist and destructive to
classroom community” (McCoach et al., 2006, p.339), and that it often hurts students’
self-confidence, motivation, and self-concepts (Chorzempa & Graham, 2006;
McCoach et al., 2006; Routman, 2003).
Let’s browse through specific findings from numerous
peer-reviewed empirical studies to see whether the effects of ability grouping
are equitable for all students, both educationally
and socially (the goal of education)…
Let’s Dig Deeper into the Research (there’s a lot, so be patient)…
*Many
findings could be in more than one category, so please look through all of
them!
Racial Gaps Related to Ability Grouping- Which Groups are Affected?
African-American,
Hispanic, male, and students of lower socioeconomic status (SES), have been
found, on average, to be placed significantly
and disproportionately lower in
ability-based groups than White, female, students (Condron, 2008; Tach
& Farkas, 2006).
Despite
efforts to reduce educational inequality in the United States, substantial
racial gaps in achievement and attainment remain. (Fryer & Levitt,
2006)
Ability
grouping practices are particularly detrimental to the early reading
trajectories of African American and Hispanic students who are lower grouped in first and third
grade. (Lleras & Rangel, 2009)
While
controlling for family socioeconomic status completely eliminates black-white
reading achievement gaps at school entry, these gaps reemerge during the first
few years of elementary school and are very prevalent by third grade.
(Fryer & Levitt, 2006)
One study looked at whether ability groups
reduce, reproduce, or exacerbate preexisting inequality in academic skills and
found that ability grouping actually
exacerbates inequality (Condron, 2008).
Tach
and Farkas (2006) found that group differences increase as students move up
through grades.
Black
and white students in desegregated classrooms remain socially segregated unless
school authorities create an environment that deemphasizes racial differences
and supports and promotes cross-race interactions. (Hallinan &
Teixeira, 1987)
White students are more likely to choose
black students as friends if they are in the same ability group. The
absence of academic status increases the likelihood that white students will
select black students as their best friends. (Hallinan & Teixeira,
1987)
At
least half of the racial gaps in student achievement at the end of high school
would be eliminated if early gaps were reduced. (Jencks &
Phillips, 1998)
This
stresses the importance of intervention in the early grades. If group differences increase, that means
segregation increases. The less students
interact with groups that are racially/ethnically different from themselves, the
less opportunities they will have to understand, embrace, and learn to work
with diverse groups. These are important
life skills.
Most
inequality in academic skills is not related to class, race, or gender. (Downey, von Hippel,
& Broh 2004)
We know
that skin color, gender, race, etc., does not affect IQ or learning aptitude, so
then how are students’ placements determined?
Could there be any bias (intentional or not) in the placement process?
Regarding Ability Group Placement and Opportunity to Advance
According to Hoffer (1992), if there are
opportunities to move up into a higher-ranked group then the practice of ability grouping could result in increased
competition among students and improvements in overall student effort However, mobility between ability groups is often limited, which can restrict
lower-grouped students contact with their higher-ranked peers and friendship
choices (Hallinan & Sorensen, 1985).
While all the teachers said that they viewed
ability as changeable, at least to some extent, it was also found that none of them had made more than a very few changes
between the different ability groups over the past 6 months. If they really
did believe that ability was changeable, they were not behaving as if it was
and the problem of misplacement would seem likely to be a real one. (Macintyre
& Ireson, 2002)
Teachers
perceived greater differences in achievement between students in high- and low-
ability groups than really existed. (Smith & Jussim, 1998)
African
American students were found to be less likely than white students to be
reassigned to a higher ability group during the school year. (Hallinan, 1996)
Placement
of students in ability groups has been found to be based on formal and/or informal test scores and on teacher judgments
and/or observations of students’ behaviors (Chorzempa & Graham, 2006;
Tach & Farkas, 2006). What is not
typically factored into student placement are students’ strengths and
weaknesses, amount of help needed, oral reading skills, comprehension, phonemic
awareness skills, use of reading strategies, or their work ethic, ability to
adjust well, or motivation to learn (Chorzempa & Graham, 2006).
An analysis indicated that there was a considerable overlap of the scores of the children
in the high-, middle-and low-ability groups. (Macintyre & Ireson, 2002)
Many of
these findings suggest that misplacement is common. A lot of trust must be placed into
standardized test content, administration of and lack of bias in assessments,
and in teachers’ observations, perceptions, and judgments of students’ academic
aptitude and their behaviors if these are what typically determine placement
into ability groups…By the way, how is it that behavior is a determinant
(intentionally or not) of academic ability?
If students are challenged appropriately, won’t their undesirable
behaviors decrease? This certainly seems
to be the case with high-ranked (highly challenged) groups…
Differences between Low-, Middle-, and High- Ranked Ability Groups
The effects of ability grouping have been
found to be positive for students placed
into high-ranked groups (Chorzempa & Graham, 2006; Condron, 2008; Tach
& Farkas, 2006). Why? High-ranked
groups are asked more critical thinking and comprehension questions, and are
given the most opportunities to do independent reading and choose their own
reading materials (Chorzempa & Graham, 2006; Condron, 2008; Tach &
Farkas, 2006).
If
high-ranked groups are given all of these learning opportunities and motivating
options and resources, which is wonderful, then what learning opportunities are
lower-ranked groups typically given (which the above research states are
usually composed of students of color and of lower SES)…
Results indicate that African American and Hispanic students learn less over time if they are
placed in lower-ranked reading groups for instruction compared to nongrouped
students, and these finding are persistent even after controlling for
within-class differences in reading proficiency levels. (Lleras & Rangel, 2009)
Effects
of ability groups, both socially and academically, on students placed in low
ranked groups have been found in several studies to be significantly harmful and sometimes harmful for students placed in
middle ranked groups (Chorzempa & Graham, 2006; Condron, 2008; Tach &
Farkas, 2006). Why? Most lower ranked groups are given less
choice in what they read, less time for independent reading, less time to read
silently or be read-to by the teacher, less critical thinking questions, and
less expository materials than higher ranked groups, and are instead given more
workbooks/dittos, basal reading series, non-reading tasks (when in reading
groups), and specific reading skills instruction (Chorzempa & Graham,
2006).
Some
may argue that these students need these lower-order thinking assignments,
however if this is primarily what they are exposed to then how will they gain
the higher-order thinking skills and motivation needed to advance their
learning?
There
is strong evidence that students placed into low-ranked reading groups learn
significantly less than nongrouped students who probably would have been placed
into a similar group. Also,
middle-ranked students appear to have no advantage or disadvantage to being
middle-grouped compared to being in a nongrouped classroom. (Condron, 2008)
One study found that homogeneous ability grouping has a slight advantage over heterogeneous
mixed-ability grouping for middle-ranked groups, while high-ranked groups performed equally well in either type of group
and low-ranked groups performed best in
heterogeneous groups (Lou et al., 1996).
This
finding was slightly different from other findings in that the middle-ranked
groups did slightly better
in ability groups, but it still confirmed
that the low-ranked groups do better in mixed-ability groups
(heterogeneous). This is still not
equitable for all students.
Social-Emotional Effects of Ability Grouping and Teacher/Peer Perceptions
A case study of an elementary school in Texas
found that the school district’s reaction to the NCLB Act (No Child Left
Behind) was to have educators classify
students into one of three groups based on practice proficiency test
scores—“passers”, “bubble kids”, and “foundation” or “remedial”—in order to
implement a “data driven” system.
The presumption was that the “passers” would have no problem passing,
the “foundation” or “remedial” kids would probably not pass, and the “bubble”
kids would be close. The “bubble” kids were given extra
attention and learning opportunities to ensure they moved up to the proficiency
level, thus increasing teachers’ and schools’ pass rates (Booher-Jennings
2005). This is a form of “educational
triage” and some California schools have been reported using the same practice
(Rubin, 2004).
Self-fulfilling prophecies were found among students in low-ability
within-class (ability) groups and among classes that used within-class
(ability) grouping. Teachers’
perceptions mediated the relationship between level of ability grouping and
student achievement. The achievement gap among students in low-
and high-ability within-class grouping increased over the course of the year,
and teacher perceptions almost completely accounted for this pattern of
increasing differences. (Smith & Jussim, 1998)
In mixed-ability classes where ability grouping was used, there was greater variation in
children’s self-concepts than in classes where it was not, despite the fact
that there were no significant differences in ability. (Reuman, 1989)
The findings demonstrated that the mathematics
self-concepts of those in high-ability mathematics
groups were significantly higher than that of those in low-ability groups.
There was also a far from perfect
correspondence between mathematics ability scores and placement in mathematics
ability group. (Macintyre & Ireson, 2002)
The main reasons
students gave for preferring mixed
ability teaching were related to the ways that peers could help, inspire and
motivate each other while avoiding those in the lower sets becoming
stigmatized. (Hallam, Ireson, & Davies, 2004)
Most students (elementary) indicated that
they would least like to be in the lowest group (74%) while 60% wanted to be in
the highest group because it gave them status and a feeling of superiority.
Students were aware that different kinds of teaching and activities went on in
different groups. Those in the highest group were most satisfied with the
activities that they undertook while 54%
of those in the lowest group expressed dissatisfaction with the teaching
methods adopted. Most preferred
whole-class or individual reading to group work. The main reason expressed for this was related to inclusion; these
methods reduced feelings of being left out. (Devine, 1993)
For the
lower ability students, the terms used during teasing were those generally
accepted as derogatory. The greater the extent of the structured groupings, the
greater the apparent stigmatization of those in the lower groups.
Stigmatization was least in the school where mixed ability grouping was the
norm. (Hallam, Iresona, & Davies, 2004)
So now
you are aware of the numerous effects that ability-grouping, at least the way
it is most commonly practiced/structured, has on students and equality. Keep in mind that the above findings do not
necessarily mean that ability-grouping itself
has mostly negative academic and social effects, but rather that the way
ability-grouping is commonly practiced
has these effects. See the
recommendations page for some ways of preventing the various harmful effects
and inequalities of this type of grouping.
Note:
See references page for bibliography
No comments:
Post a Comment